Feature
A Classroom Investigation of Practice Effects
Practice makes perfect, or at least better. One of the most well-documented phenomena in cognitive psychology is that the speed (or accuracy, or fluency) of an activity improves with practice, that is, as the activity is repeated. This phenomenon, sometimes called the “Practice Effect,” is ubiquitous, and has been scientifically studied for over a half century (Crossman, 1959). It is also among the most important of psychological phenomena, holding broad implications for any domain involving human expertise (e.g., Martin et al., 2011; Côté et al., 2007).
The Seeker Game is a simple activity that introduces students to the most important and interesting aspects of practice effects. The game can be played with almost no preparation, and is easily modified for a wide range of ages and abilities. After describing the game, I suggest directions for discussion, focusing on the “microgenetic” analysis of change—that is, looking in detail at exactly what changes from round to round, leading to the observed practice effects. In these discussions I emphasize how change is multifactorial; that is, it does not take place only, nor even primarily inside the players’ heads, but rather straddles the cognitive, social, and physical domains. I also touch on some topics for more advanced discussion around the game.
I use The Seeker Game in my applied cognitive science course at Stanford, after students have been introduced to the basics of applied cognition, especially the concepts of problem-solving and cognitive strategies. However, I have used the game with children as young as seven years old, and one can introduce the relevant cognitive concepts through the activity itself. Below I describe the game in its simplest form. For younger students I add an adventure-style cover story.
Setup: The game requires only cardstock (or manila folders), several sets of stick-on numbers (e.g., 1–10), and a second timer that goes up to a few minutes. Nothing here depends upon high precision. I break up the class into teams of about three students each, distribute a page and sticky numbers to each team, and ask them to each create a random pattern of numbers on their page. The only requirement is that the lowest number must be placed in the upper left-hand corner of the page. The rest of the numbers should be distributed randomly about the page. Figure 1 depicts one such pattern.
Play: Team members distribute the roles of “Seeker,” “Guide,” and “Timer” among themselves. The main phase of the game is played in about six rounds or trials. The goal of the seeker is to touch each of the numbers in order, beginning at 1 and ending at 10. The timer will time how long each round takes. What makes this interesting is this: The seeker must keep their eyes closed the entire time, therefore the guide must help the seeker reach the goal. However, the guide may not touch the seeker or the page, so the seeker and guide must collaborate to get the seeker through the pattern, touching each number in order, using voice direction or some other sort of auditory signal.
In practice, each round works like this: At the beginning of the round, the guide moves the seeker’s hand to the first number, on the upper left of the page. This is the only case where the guide can touch the seeker. When signaled by the timer to start, the guide then guides the seeker to touch each number, in order, as quickly and accurately as possible, interacting only in voice or auditory signals. If the seeker misses a number, the guide must get the seeker to loop back to it so that the numbers are touched in order. When the seeker reaches the last number, the timer records the time for that trial, and the team sets up for the next round. The seeker must not look at the pattern until all rounds are completed.
After ensuring that everyone understands the instructions, I ask all the seekers to close their eyes. At this point I ask the teams to trade their pages around, so that each team now has a new pattern that their seeker has not seen. This minimizes the seeker being helped by a latent memory of the pattern that their own team created.
Recording results: While the teams work, I prepare a graph on the board where the independent (x) axis indicates the round or trial number, and the dependent (y) axis records time in minutes/seconds (Figure 2). Rarely does a trial take more than three minutes. Although there are only five or six rounds in the game, my x axis goes out several additional rounds. These will be used in a transfer phase, described below.
As each team finishes all the rounds, plot their times. Eventually the graph should look something like the left part of Figures 2 and 3.
Inviting students to observe the data, the most obvious phenomenon will be that every team overall gets faster, although their paths will, of course, differ, and there are likely to be some points of slowing. The greatest speedup is usually in the early trials, with the speedup decelerating as practice progresses. This is the famous “power law of practice,” although, as discussed later, practice effects may not really fit a power function. I often emphasize this by drawing an “eyeball power curve” through the data (dashed blue curve in Figure 2).
What changes from round to round, leading every team to speed up, and why does the learning take the observed shape? To explore these questions I lead the class in a microgenetic analysis (Siegler, 1987, 1995; Agre & Shrager, 1990) by eliciting detailed descriptions of how each team’s procedure changed from one round to the next.
The first things that will come to mind are cognitive changes in the seeker. For example, the seeker’s memory of the pattern is building up, and they are learning to anticipate where to go next; they are “getting it into their head and hands,” so to speak, a phenomenon sometimes (too simplistically) called “motor memory.” However, it can easily be observed that learning is multifactorial; there are many changes in the strategy employed by the seeker and guide, both individually and as a team. Some of the most obvious changes will be in the communications between seeker and guide. For example, players commonly run into a “left ↔ right” confusion, resulting from the seeker and guide having different points of view, especially if they are sitting across from one another. Most teams facing this sort of problem quickly figure out how to resolve it by using compass points, for example, or more simply moving to a shoulder-to-shoulder arrangement. Another class of common strategic improvements surrounds how the team resolves errors. Importantly, even changes that appear to take place purely in the head (or “hands”) of the seeker rapidly lead to changes in the structure of team collaboration. For example, some seekers learn to use several fingers, or even their whole hand, to find the next number by feel. The discovery of this new sensory facility enables improved efficiency in the guide’s directions. For example, the guide may now only have to give rough direction, being able to lean on the seeker’s new capability.
Open-ended microgenetic analysis, that is, discussion of other changes experienced by the participants, should be encouraged, and occupy a significant period of time. Given space constraints, I cannot go into very many of the changes that will commonly be discovered once the class becomes engaged in microgenetic analysis, but it is worth drawing out the core concepts that should arise:
An additional phase that I generally add to the basic activity, time allowing, is an exploration of transfer effects. That is, how performance changes when aspects of the task are changed. One could ask the teams to swap the seeker and guide roles, and/or have the teams swap patterns. These manipulations will have a greater or lesser effect of making the activity temporarily more difficult, increasing the rate of mistakes, and the time it takes to complete the task.
I usually do only two or three additional trials with the new configuration, and the right part of Figure 3 depicts actual results in one class. Roughly, the task completion times increase, but not all the way back up to where they were at the start, and rapidly return to their asymptotic march toward the physical limit. This happens because the new task shares much in common with the former task. The advantage deriving from reuse of learnings supported by common elements between tasks is variously called “savings,” “learnings,” or “positive transfer,” and the extra load, and thus lag, resulting from changed, uncommon elements is variously called “loss” or “negative transfer” (Singley & Anderson, 1989).
A typical savings across changes is that the team has already figured out a good communicative strategy, although the particular players might not have practiced these in their new roles. Also, even if the roles are swapped, if the patterns are not swapped between teams, the former guide (who now becomes seeker), will have some memory of the pattern having guided the previous seeker through it several times, although they will not have the “motor memory” that the original seeker had. Notice that if the pattern is changed, the seeker’s memory of the pattern is likely to actually interfere with learning the new one, a source of “loss” (sometimes called “interference”). The more the elements of the task are changed, the more error-prone and slower the activity will become, although learning continues to operate, and performance will continue to improve from the change point forward.
Having observed that strategy change constitutes the greatest part of the practice effect, at least early on, it is natural to ask where strategy changes come from. Unfortunately, like the path contingency observed above, strategy change is also fundamentally contingent. Just as there are many paths to similar endpoints, and which path a person or group takes will depend on tiny details of the participants’ experience and of the learning context, how and when these paths are discovered is fundamentally unpredictable.
People might invent new, efficient strategies on their own, as the teams do in this activity. But in the real world, it is much more common to have strategies culturally communicated: Your piano teacher tells you to hold your hands a particular way, and organizes the pieces you play to build up certain skills; a baseball coach tells you to keep your eye on the ball; your flight instructor tells you to scan your instruments in a particular order; coaches tell us not to practice our mistakes; tutors teach us to use spaced practice; your parents teach you to say “please” and “thank you,” and to look both ways before crossing the street; and so on. Indeed, teaching, coaching, and parenting are all about offering us strategies and guiding us through experiences in an order that will make learning safe and efficient.
Here are some ideas for more advanced discussion, given more time or a more advanced course:
Example results, in addition to the per-team plots. The blue dashed line is an “eyeball average,” and the blue horizontal line at the bottom is an eye-ball asymptotic limit, estimating the fastest possible tour through the numbers.
A different in-class run of the experiment. The rightmost two repeats depict savings/loss results after exchange of patterns and roles.
Humans are incredible; with only a few simple instructions, even young children are able to succeed and improve in novel, complex activities. We often think of learning in simple terms: “Practice Makes Perfect,” or asserted “laws” such as “The Power Law of Practice.” Microgenetic analysis, looking closely at the sources of change, reveals a complex interaction among the cognitive, physical, and interpersonal elements of human activity. The models that arise from these sorts of analyses have important implications for human performance, as well as in the practical design of educational and coaching interventions, and the engineering of any sort of system that involves people.
The phenomena of practice effects are directly applicable to any domain involving humans. Not only do we communicate strategies to one another through teaching, coaching, and parenting, we design them into our engineered world. Every engineered system embodies a preferred mode of use, and it is expected by the designers that either people can immediately use the device efficiently, or, more commonly, that they can efficiently learn to use it. Indeed, the course that uses this activity is called “Interaction Analysis,” and it is a capstone seminar in the Human-Computer Interaction track within the Symbolic Systems Program—approximately the Cognitive Science department—at Stanford. This course is generally attended by senior and early graduate students in symbolic systems, computer science, psychology, engineering design, and a smattering of students from the “hard” sciences.
Microgenetic analysis of The Seeker Game helps us to see how change straddles the psychological, physical, and social context; when something changes in human activity, the change may be in one person’s head, but most changes involve both individuals and the world, and often involve social connections. The fact that psychological phenomena often occupy a middle-ground between the cognitive, social, and physical domains is one of the aspects that makes psychology a particularly difficult science. Indeed, modern statistics was, in large part, invented to deal with the complexity of psychology. I sometimes joke with my colleagues that the richness and complexity of important human phenomena, such as the practice effect, and the dangers of abstracting “laws” over complex behavioral phenomena, is what makes the “soft” sciences the hard ones! ■
I thank the students in my Symbolic Systems 245 course at Stanford, over many years, for their patience and good humor in putting up with playing The Seeker Game, and in helping to improve it. I thank two anonymous reviewers, as well as Dr. Matthew Lewis, and Dr. David Wojnowski for guidance that significantly improved this paper.
Jeff Shrager (jshrager@stanford.edu) is co-founder and director of research at xCures, Inc. in San Francisco, California, and an adjunct professor in the Symbolic Systems Program at Stanford University in Stanford, California.
Biology Phenomena Teaching Strategies Postsecondary